Education-publishing giant Pearson recently tested 鈥渟ocial-psychological鈥 messages in learning software used by more than 9,000 unwitting students at 165 U.S. colleges and universities, highlighting the emerging hopes and fears about the ways the ed-tech industry might seek to capitalize on recent research into the impact of students鈥 mindsets on their learning, including in K-12.
The study, 鈥淓mbedding Research-Inspired Innovations in EdTech: An RCT of Social-Psychological Interventions, at Scale,鈥 was presented last month at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association.
The release of the research prompted a fierce debate over issues of ethics, privacy, and consent during large-scale testing of such strategies using commercial software programs. Pearson鈥檚 stock fell noticeably in response related to concerns, which the company described as unwarranted.
Sharp debates about privacy, consent, and ethics spilled into public view following the release of a study in which education publisher Pearson used commercial software to test strategies for changing students鈥 mindsets.
Among the questions raised:
- Where is the line between product improvement and clinical research?
- When does something like promoting a 鈥済rowth mindset鈥 or seeking to change students鈥 attitudes become a psychological experiment?
- What kind of consent can and should be obtained before testing 鈥渟ocial-psychological interventions鈥 in commercial learning software?
Justin Reich, the executive director of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology鈥檚 Teaching Systems Lab, addresses those questions on 澳门跑狗论坛鈥檚 EdTech Researcher blog.
鈥淓very educational software company and publisher will be modifying their products over time to try to improve them,鈥 Reich writes. 鈥淚鈥檇 like to incentivize them to do so in a way that the public benefits from those companies sharing what they learn.鈥
Read the full post here.
鈥淩ecent media coverage has mischaracterized this relatively minor product update by extracting technical language from the research that, when taken out of context, and paired with words like 鈥榚xperiment,鈥 conveys a malicious intent,鈥 said Scott Overland, Pearson鈥檚 director of media relations, in a statement.
Through its study, Pearson sought to determine whether encouraging a 鈥済rowth mindset鈥 and grounding students鈥 expectations of how much effort a task might require would lead them to attempt and complete more problems. The test took place in a popular commercial-software program called MyLab Programming, often used for college-level introductory computer-programming courses.
Pearson embedded the messaging into some versions of the software, then randomly assigned different colleges to use different versions of the program. The company did not seek prior consent from participating institutions or individuals.
The results included some modest signs that growth-mindset messaging can increase students鈥 persistence when they start a problem, then run into difficulty. That鈥檚 likely to bolster proponents of the idea, who say it鈥檚 important to encourage students to view intelligence as something that can change with practice and hard work.
But students who received both types of social-psychological messages tested by Pearson attempted fewer problems than their peers, running counter to the researchers鈥 expectations.
Outside experts offered skeptical reactions, on a number of fronts.
鈥淚t does not surprise me at all that corporations are attempting to monetize a promising way of thinking about a hairy problem,鈥 said Phi Delta Kappan CEO Joshua Starr, who was a major proponent of social-emotional learning during his time as superintendent of the Montgomery County, Md., school district and who currently serves on the Aspen Institute鈥檚 National Commission on Social, Emotional, and Academic Development.
鈥淭here is some value鈥 to Pearson鈥檚 approach, Starr said, but 鈥渟ocial-emotional learning is best promoted through strong communities and relationships,鈥 not software.
And Ben Williamson, a lecturer at the University of Stirling in the United Kingdom who studies big data in education, raised other concerns.
There鈥檚 little evidence that focusing on growth mindset in the classroom will significantly benefit students, Williamson argued, citing recent analyses finding limited effects of mindset-based interventions in K-12 classrooms.
In addition, Williamson maintained, companies such as Pearson would be wise to pay close attention to the growing public anxiety over the ways companies collect people鈥檚 sensitive information and use it for psychological profiling and targeting.
鈥淚t鈥檚 concerning that forms of low-level psychological experimentation to trigger certain behaviors appears to be happening in the ed-tech sector, and students might not know those experiments are taking place,鈥 Williamson said.
In an interview, Kristen DiCerbo, Pearson鈥檚 vice president for learning research and design, described the experiment as part of the company鈥檚 鈥減roduct-improvement process"鈥攁n early test to see if new mindset-messaging features actually work, before they are rolled out comprehensively.
Utilizing commercial software allowed Pearson to see how the changes played out for real students and actual classrooms, DiCerbo said, generating more useful information than had it taken place in a lab鈥攐r if the company had rolled out new messaging without systematically studying its impact at all.
Pearson also noted that the study did not involve the collection of any personally identifiable information on individual students and that federal guidelines don鈥檛 require consent for research involving 鈥渘ormal educational practice.鈥
鈥淲e think these motivational aspects are really important for students鈥 learning outcomes,鈥 DiCerbo said.
鈥淏ut the only way we鈥檙e going to know for sure is to do the research.鈥
Mixed Results
Pearson鈥檚 MyLab Programming software is typically used for introductory computer-science courses where students learn the programming languages C++ or Java.
DiCerbo said that made sense as the first content area to test social-psychological messaging, because many students have a propensity to attribute failure in programming to a personal shortcoming, rather than seeing it as a challenge and opportunity to learn.
The idea was to see if students鈥 motivation and achievement would be improved in either of two ways:
- Inserting 鈥済rowth-mindset鈥 messages (stressing the importance of effort and building skills over time) into the software鈥檚 instructions and into the feedback it offered to students who provided wrong answers. An example: 鈥淣o one is born a great programmer. Success takes hours and hours of practice.鈥
- Using 鈥渁nchoring of effort鈥 messages (seeking to leverage a common cognitive bias in which people tend to rely on the first piece of information they learn, even if it鈥檚 irrelevant to the problem they鈥檙e trying to solve). Pearson鈥檚 theory was that students might not have any sense of how much effort is often required to solve computer-programming problems, so providing them with a high-end estimate based on analysis of previous users鈥 experience could ground them in the expectation that multiple attempts would be necessary. An example: 鈥淪ome students tried this question 26 times! Don鈥檛 worry if it takes you a few tries to get it right.鈥
The researchers were surprised to learn that students who didn鈥檛 receive any special messaging from the software tried to solve significantly more problems (212) than those who received growth-mindset messages (174) or anchoring messages (156).
That finding suggested that the social-psychological interventions they were testing backfired, although DiCerbo said other factors鈥攅specially differences in how various instructors use the software in their classes鈥攎ay have also played a role.
鈥極pen Question鈥
But the Pearson team also found that students who received the growth-mindset messages successfully completed more of the problems they started than their counterparts. Those students were also significantly more likely to eventually solve problems they initially got incorrect, supporting the idea that encouraging a growth mindset can have positive benefits when students run into difficulty.
Big-picture, DiCerbo said, there鈥檚 still a lot of work to be done鈥攁nd Pearson, which is preparing to sell its K-12 curriculum business, believes that more such tests may be valuable.
鈥淚t鈥檚 still an open question as to whether technology is even capable of providing this type of feedback,鈥 DiCerbo said.