Those looking for what works in education will find no shortage of advice. Educators, hoping to improve student outcomes, eagerly embrace recommendations telling them to 鈥淐ater to each child鈥檚 learning style!鈥 and 鈥淕ive students awards for positive behaviors!鈥 But many such intuitive, popular 鈥渂est practices鈥 may not, in fact, be what is best for students, even though their proponents stamp them 鈥渆vidence based.鈥
Educators who prioritize evidence-based practices are fighting an uphill battle; the standards of proof for what constitutes 鈥渆vidence鈥 in schools鈥攁nd education more widely鈥攁re often exceedingly low. For instance, the popular notions that we should be teaching to students鈥 or providing students with were both rooted in observational evidence. Both practices have now been debunked, but over 75 percent of educators still endorse learning styles, and many schools say they use awards to recognize excellent student attendance.
We should not be surprised when people have incorrect notions on what research says works鈥攅ducation research is littered with on a range of practical topics that either do not replicate previous findings or suggest massively inflated effects.
Educational policymakers and practitioners need to understand how study designs and research practices influence the reproducibility and credibility of a study鈥檚 findings. This is easier said than done, but there are a couple of initial indicators that suggest a research finding is 鈥渞eal鈥 and worth implementing.
First, to disentangle whether a practice causes improvement or is merely associated with it, we need to use research methods that can reliably identify causal relationships. And the best way to determine whether a practice causes an outcome is to conduct a randomized controlled trial (or 鈥淩CT,鈥 meaning participants were randomly assigned to being exposed to the practice under study or not being exposed to it).
The standards of proof for what constitutes 'evidence' in schools鈥攁nd education more widely鈥攁re often exceedingly low."
Second, policymakers and practitioners evaluating research studies should have more confidence in studies where the same findings have been observed multiple times in different settings with large samples. Many educational practices are based on single research studies with small sample sizes. We can learn from small, one-off studies. But when it comes to adopting practices, we recommend those that have been evaluated in studies with large sample sizes and reproducible results.
Finally, we can have much more faith in a study鈥檚 findings when they are . That is, researchers publicly post exactly what their hypotheses are and exactly how they will evaluate each one before they have examined their data. This helps limit flexible research practices, making it less likely that researchers will find statistically significant results by chance.
Some will lament that large RCTs are too expensive, slow, or difficult to implement, and that preregistering studies is not feasible because educational research is messy and unpredictable. Yet several studies conducted in the past few years prove that changes in educational practice evaluated by large-scale, reproducible, preregistered RCTs exist and can be used to inform work on the ground.
In our own work, we have spent the last six years studying how to reduce student absenteeism. Through two large-scale, preregistered RCTs ( with more than 28,000 K-12 students and with almost 11,000 K-5 students), our research team found that sending mailings to parents several times over the course of the school year with personalized attendance information that dynamically targets key parental misbeliefs consistently reduces chronic absenteeism 10 percent to 15 percent. This research led to the creation of , a program that partners with districts around the country to help them reduce student absenteeism by implementing this research-backed intervention.
Another practice that educators can鈥攁nd, strong evidence suggests, should鈥攖ake up comes from a study conducted by Peter Bergman and Eric Chan. They randomly assigned parents of more than 1,000 middle and high school students in 22 schools to receive automated, frequent information via text message about their child鈥檚 missed assignments and grades. The study, which has now been replicated multiple times, found that this strategy led to a 28 percent reduction in course failures, a 12 percent increase in specific class attendance, and increased student retention by 1.5 percentage points. Providing parents with information that helps them monitor their child鈥檚 academic progress can have meaningful impacts on student success.
Finally, a led by David Yeager explored for whom growth mindset interventions are most effective. In a nationally representative sample of more than 12,000 students, the study found that adolescents assigned to complete a 鈥済rowth mindset鈥 intervention鈥攚hich taught that intellectual abilities can be developed鈥攅arned higher GPAs (a modest, but real 0.05-grade points) in core classes at the end of the 9th grade. The authors preregistered that they predicted the intervention would . Consistent with this, they found that low-achieving students showed larger effects with higher GPAs (0.10-grade points) in core classes at the end of the 9th grade and were 11 percentage points less likely to get a D or F average in one of these classes. This large-scale and preregistered study provides evidence that mindset interventions can improve outcomes for struggling students.
Each of these relatively low-cost and easy-to-implement interventions have modest but real impacts on student outcomes. Holding educational research to greater standards of evidence will very likely mean the effect sizes that are reported will be smaller. But they will reflect reality.
Expectations about how much impact interventions tend to have need to be massively recalibrated since more-rigorous research with larger sample sizes tends to find smaller effect sizes. If an educational intervention鈥檚 outcomes seem too good to be true, they probably are. (A recent by Matt Kraft of Brown University provides a helpful overview of how we might think about effect sizes in education.)
There do not appear to be single 鈥渟ilver bullets鈥 that will easily equalize and accelerate educational outcomes. The reality is that educational gains will come from a combination of many well-supported, evidence-based practices and communities of caring adults helping kids.
We hope educational policymakers and practitioners will start proactively looking for practices that meet these standards of evidence and work to widely implement them. By doing so, we can move steadily toward greater educational success for all students.